Show simple item record

[journal article]

dc.contributor.authorJohnston, Jane Susande
dc.date.accessioned2010-11-26T03:18:00Zde
dc.date.accessioned2012-08-29T23:00:38Z
dc.date.available2012-08-29T23:00:38Z
dc.date.issued2009de
dc.identifier.urihttp://www.ssoar.info/ssoar/handle/document/20727
dc.description.abstractFifty six children, aged between four and eleven years of age, in seven groups, were videoed playing with, being questioned about and sorting a collection of toys in order to identify what skills of observation looked like in young children, how observations influenced other scientific skills and what supported the skill of observation. Children's skills of observation were found to be similar across all ages and included affective, functional, social and exploratory comments, actions and questions. These initial observations led to the use of other scientific process skills: classification, prediction, hypotheses, along with explanation for younger children and interpretations for older children. There was generally a greater sophistication of observation skills with increasing age of the children. Observation in young children was found to be tactile and developed in two ways; by engaging in more unique close observation and interpreting observation by utilising previous knowledge and experiences. Important factors affecting the development of observational and other scientific skills were found to be the context (activity, environment, resources) and combination of social interactions between individuals, peers and adults. This combination supported the development of both observational and other scientific skills, although the nature and amount of this interaction appeared individual to different groups of children and could not be predicted.en
dc.languageende
dc.subject.ddcPsychologyen
dc.subject.ddcPsychologiede
dc.subject.otherEarly years; Primary school; Process skills
dc.titleWhat does the skill of observation look like in young children?en
dc.description.reviewbegutachtet (peer reviewed)de
dc.description.reviewpeer revieweden
dc.source.journalInternational Journal of Science Educationde
dc.source.volume31de
dc.publisher.countryGBR
dc.source.issue18de
dc.subject.classozAllgemeine Psychologiede
dc.subject.classozGeneral Psychologyen
dc.subject.thesozsurveillanceen
dc.subject.thesozObservationde
dc.identifier.urnurn:nbn:de:0168-ssoar-207271de
dc.date.modified2011-09-01T13:26:00Zde
dc.rights.licencePEER Licence Agreement (applicable only to documents from PEER project)de
dc.rights.licencePEER Licence Agreement (applicable only to documents from PEER project)en
ssoar.gesis.collectionSOLIS;ADISde
ssoar.contributor.institutionhttp://www.peerproject.eu/de
internal.status3de
internal.identifier.thesoz10053508
dc.type.stockarticlede
dc.type.documentjournal articleen
dc.type.documentZeitschriftenartikelde
dc.rights.copyrightfde
dc.source.pageinfo2511-2525
internal.identifier.classoz10703
internal.identifier.journal171de
internal.identifier.document32
internal.identifier.ddc150
dc.identifier.doihttps://doi.org/10.1080/09500690802644637de
dc.description.pubstatusPostprinten
dc.description.pubstatusPostprintde
internal.identifier.licence7
internal.identifier.pubstatus2
internal.identifier.review1
internal.check.abstractlanguageharmonizerCERTAIN
internal.check.languageharmonizerCERTAIN_RETAINED


Files in this item

Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record